How difficult is chimpanzee termite fishing?
What depth of intellect and manual dexterity are required? Suppose you are
dropped naked into the Gombe Preserve in Tanzania and, like it or not, discover
that termites are your principal hedge against malnutrition or starvation.
You know they're an excellent source of protein; you know that self-respecting
humans in many parts of the world regularly eat them. You manage to put
aside whatever compunctions you may feel. But catching them one at a time
is not going to be worth the effort. Unless you're lucky enough to encounter
them when they're swarming, you're going to have to make a tool, repeatedly
insert it into their meter-high mound, introduce the tool into your mouth,
and strip off the clinging termites with your teeth and lips as you withdraw
the tool from your mouth. Could you do as well as a chimp?
The anthropologist Geza Teleki tried to find out. He spent months in Gombe
under the tutelage of a chimp named Leakey, who was adept at the technique.
Teleki wrote about his findings in a famous scientific paper called "Chimpanzee
Subsistence Technology.'' The Gombe termites mainly come out at night; before
dawn they expertly wall up all the entrances to their mounds. Chimps routinely
begin their termite foraging by scraping away these entrance barriers. Teleki's
inquiry started there:
Having repeatedly observed [chimpanzee] individuals approach a mound, make
a rapid visual scan of the surface while standing on or beside it, and reach
decisively outwith a high degree of predictive accuracyto uncover a tunnel,
I was soon impressed by the apparent ease with which tunnels could be located.
In attempting to learn the technique, I applied several experimental procedures:
examining in minute detail all crack patterns, protuberances, depressions
and other "topographic" features in the clay. But, after weeks
of futile searching for the essential clue, I had to resort to scraping
mound surfaces with a jackknife until a tunnel was inadvertently exposed.
My inability to find any physical features which could serve as visual clues
eventually led me to realize that chimpanzees may possess knowledge far
beyond my expectations.. . .
The only hypothesis which, at this point, seems to reasonably account for
the observed facts is that an adult chimpanzee may know (memorize?) the
precise location of two or more tunnels in the most familiar mounds. Moreover,
since intensive probing is restricted to a short annual season, the possibility
that chimpanzees retain a mental map of core mound features during the intervening
months must also be considered. That chimpanzees require a prolonged learning
period (i.e. 4-5 years) to gain proficiency in this technique . . ., and
that some individuals are known to have the capability to retain specific
information for many years, provides circumstantial support for this hypothesis.
Next, Teleki looked into a selection of raw materials for the manufacture
of the termite probe:
When performed by experienced chimpanzees, the selection procedure seems
deceptively simple. After a brief visual scan of the nearby vegetation,
a chimpanzee will usually extend a hand and deftly tear off a twig, vine
or grass stalk. Sometimes the individual must move a few paces away from
the mound and fetch a suitable probe, and in some cases 2-3 objects are
initially selected. These may be rapidly examined and discarded until some
specification is met in one, or several may be carried to the mound for
subsequent selection. Whenever it occurs, the selection is made in a swift,
almost casual manner, and modification is begun if necessary. Without being
aware of the nuances involved, it is easy to undervalue the proficiency
needed to perform these maneuvers.
Chimpanzees presumably have the experience whereby the properties of an
object can be evaluated before it is applied to the task of probing, for
the rate of error in selecting probes is not high. . . When probing for
termites, the specifications are in fact surprisingly stringent: if the
vine or grass selected is too pliant, it will buckle and collapse (accordion-like
when inserted into a twisted tunnel; if, on the other hand, the object is
too stiff or brittle, it will catch on the tunnel walls and either break
or resist entry to the necessary depth....
Despite months of observing and aping adult chimpanzees as they selected
probes with enviable ease, speed and accuracy, I was unable to achieve their
level of competence. Similar ineptness can only be observed in chimpanzees
below the age of about 4-5 years.
Finally, putting aside the difficulties in finding the tunnel entrances
and manufacturing the tools, Teleki set himself to learning how to use a
competently produced tool:
I spent many hours inserting probes, pausing for the designated interval,
and pulling them out againwithout getting any termites. Only after some
weeks of nearly total failure . . . did I finally begin to grasp the problems
involved . . .In order to collect these subterranean termites, the probing
object must first be carefully and dexterously inserted to a depth of about
8-16 cm [centimeters], with appropriate turns of the wrist so that the object
navigates the twisting channel. The probe must then be gently vibrated with
the fingers during the prescribed pause, for without this movement the termites
may not be stimulated into biting firmly onto the probe. However, if the
vibration is performed too lengthily or roughly, there is an excellent chance
that the probe will be cut through by the [termites'] mandibles while still
in the tunnel. When these preliminary actions have been correctly performed,
the probe, presumably with dozens of termites now attached, must be extracted
from the tunnel. Once again there are nuances to be observed. If the object
is too rapidly or clumsily pulled out, the insects are likely to be scraped
off along the sides of the tunnel, which then yields nothing but a shredded
probe. The hand motions must be reasonably but not overly swift and, once
started, uniformly fluid and graceful. If the tunnel is particularly tortuous
(a feature which can be determined during insertion of the probe), the success
of the catch can be ensured by a slow twisting of the wrist while the probe
is pulled out.